Scores on benchmarks
Model rank shown below is with respect to all public models..127 |
average_vision
rank 366
81 benchmarks |
|
.205 |
neural_vision
rank 359
38 benchmarks |
|
.081 |
V1
rank 397
24 benchmarks |
|
.045 |
Marques2020
[reference]
rank 349
22 benchmarks |
|
.099 |
V1-orientation
rank 355
7 benchmarks |
|
.696 |
Marques2020_Ringach2002-circular_variance
v1
rank 266
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.216 |
V1-response_selectivity
rank 347
4 benchmarks |
|
.866 |
Marques2020_FreemanZiemba2013-texture_variance_ratio
v1
[reference]
rank 75
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.198 |
FreemanZiemba2013.V1-pls
v2
[reference]
rank 405
|
|
recordings from
102
sites in
V1
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.146 |
V2
rank 292
2 benchmarks |
|
.293 |
FreemanZiemba2013.V2-pls
v2
[reference]
rank 246
|
|
recordings from
103
sites in
V2
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.338 |
V4
rank 316
5 benchmarks |
|
.443 |
SanghaviJozwik2020.V4-pls
v1
[reference]
rank 258
|
|
recordings from
50
sites in
V4
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.561 |
Sanghavi2020.V4-pls
v1
[reference]
rank 318
|
|
recordings from
47
sites in
V4
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.178 |
SanghaviMurty2020.V4-pls
v1
[reference]
rank 267
|
|
recordings from
46
sites in
V4
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.509 |
MajajHong2015.V4-pls
v3
[reference]
rank 364
|
|
recordings from
88
sites in
V4
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.256 |
IT
rank 283
7 benchmarks |
|
.356 |
SanghaviMurty2020.IT-pls
v1
[reference]
rank 178
|
|
recordings from
29
sites in
IT
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.486 |
Sanghavi2020.IT-pls
v1
[reference]
rank 290
|
|
recordings from
88
sites in
IT
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.493 |
SanghaviJozwik2020.IT-pls
v1
[reference]
rank 171
|
|
recordings from
26
sites in
IT
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.457 |
MajajHong2015.IT-pls
v3
[reference]
rank 311
|
|
recordings from
168
sites in
IT
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
X |
Kar2019-ost
v2
[reference]
rank X
|
|
recordings from
424
sites in
IT
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.048 |
behavior_vision
rank 353
43 benchmarks |
|
.330 |
Rajalingham2018-i2n
v2
[reference]
rank 313
|
|
match-to-sample task
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.053 |
Geirhos2021-error_consistency
[reference]
rank 278
17 benchmarks |
|
.006 |
Geirhos2021colour-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 292
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.016 |
Geirhos2021contrast-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 284
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.098 |
Geirhos2021cueconflict-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 254
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.079 |
Geirhos2021edge-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 149
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.125 |
Geirhos2021eidolonI-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 252
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.112 |
Geirhos2021eidolonII-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 251
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.081 |
Geirhos2021eidolonIII-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 268
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.035 |
Geirhos2021falsecolour-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 252
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.055 |
Geirhos2021highpass-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 140
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.026 |
Geirhos2021lowpass-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 274
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.011 |
Geirhos2021phasescrambling-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 276
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.007 |
Geirhos2021powerequalisation-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 285
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.035 |
Geirhos2021rotation-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 264
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.098 |
Geirhos2021silhouette-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 263
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.029 |
Geirhos2021sketch-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 263
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.040 |
Geirhos2021stylized-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 275
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.049 |
Geirhos2021uniformnoise-error_consistency
v1
[reference]
rank 197
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.215 |
engineering_vision
rank 227
25 benchmarks |
|
.455 |
ImageNet-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 220
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.185 |
ImageNet-C-top1
[reference]
rank 189
4 benchmarks |
|
.115 |
ImageNet-C-noise-top1
v2
[reference]
rank 192
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.163 |
ImageNet-C-blur-top1
v2
[reference]
rank 189
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.188 |
ImageNet-C-weather-top1
v2
[reference]
rank 190
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.271 |
ImageNet-C-digital-top1
v2
[reference]
rank 184
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.040 |
ObjectNet-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 122
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.394 |
Geirhos2021-top1
[reference]
rank 235
17 benchmarks |
|
.808 |
Geirhos2021colour-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 234
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.430 |
Geirhos2021contrast-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 218
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.214 |
Geirhos2021cueconflict-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 119
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.338 |
Geirhos2021edge-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 66
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.470 |
Geirhos2021eidolonI-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 190
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.395 |
Geirhos2021eidolonII-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 242
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.390 |
Geirhos2021eidolonIII-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 241
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.700 |
Geirhos2021falsecolour-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 239
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.200 |
Geirhos2021highpass-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 231
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.290 |
Geirhos2021lowpass-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 238
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.430 |
Geirhos2021phasescrambling-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 235
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.339 |
Geirhos2021powerequalisation-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 237
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.435 |
Geirhos2021rotation-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 233
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.306 |
Geirhos2021silhouette-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 234
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.380 |
Geirhos2021sketch-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 238
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.287 |
Geirhos2021stylized-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 225
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
.291 |
Geirhos2021uniformnoise-top1
v1
[reference]
rank 200
|
|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
How to use
from brainscore_vision import load_model model = load_model("mobilenet_v1_0.25_160") model.start_task(...) model.start_recording(...) model.look_at(...)
Benchmarks bibtex
@article {Marques2021.03.01.433495, author = {Marques, Tiago and Schrimpf, Martin and DiCarlo, James J.}, title = {Multi-scale hierarchical neural network models that bridge from single neurons in the primate primary visual cortex to object recognition behavior}, elocation-id = {2021.03.01.433495}, year = {2021}, doi = {10.1101/2021.03.01.433495}, publisher = {Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory}, abstract = {Primate visual object recognition relies on the representations in cortical areas at the top of the ventral stream that are computed by a complex, hierarchical network of neural populations. While recent work has created reasonably accurate image-computable hierarchical neural network models of those neural stages, those models do not yet bridge between the properties of individual neurons and the overall emergent behavior of the ventral stream. One reason we cannot yet do this is that individual artificial neurons in multi-stage models have not been shown to be functionally similar to individual biological neurons. Here, we took an important first step by building and evaluating hundreds of hierarchical neural network models in how well their artificial single neurons approximate macaque primary visual cortical (V1) neurons. We found that single neurons in certain models are surprisingly similar to their biological counterparts and that the distributions of single neuron properties, such as those related to orientation and spatial frequency tuning, approximately match those in macaque V1. Critically, we observed that hierarchical models with V1 stages that better match macaque V1 at the single neuron level are also more aligned with human object recognition behavior. Finally, we show that an optimized classical neuroscientific model of V1 is more functionally similar to primate V1 than all of the tested multi-stage models, suggesting room for further model improvements with tangible payoffs in closer alignment to human behavior. These results provide the first multi-stage, multi-scale models that allow our field to ask precisely how the specific properties of individual V1 neurons relate to recognition behavior.HighlightsImage-computable hierarchical neural network models can be naturally extended to create hierarchical {\textquotedblleft}brain models{\textquotedblright} that allow direct comparison with biological neural networks at multiple scales {\textendash} from single neurons, to population of neurons, to behavior.Single neurons in some of these hierarchical brain models are functionally similar to single neurons in macaque primate visual cortex (V1)Some hierarchical brain models have processing stages in which the entire distribution of artificial neuron properties closely matches the biological distributions of those same properties in macaque V1Hierarchical brain models whose V1 processing stages better match the macaque V1 stage also tend to be more aligned with human object recognition behavior at their output stageCompeting Interest StatementThe authors have declared no competing interest.}, URL = {https://www.biorxiv.org/content/early/2021/08/13/2021.03.01.433495}, eprint = {https://www.biorxiv.org/content/early/2021/08/13/2021.03.01.433495.full.pdf}, journal = {bioRxiv} } @article{Freeman2013, author = {Freeman, Jeremy and Ziemba, Corey M. and Heeger, David J. and Simoncelli, E. P. and Movshon, J. A.}, doi = {10.1038/nn.3402}, issn = {10976256}, journal = {Nature Neuroscience}, number = {7}, pages = {974--981}, pmid = {23685719}, publisher = {Nature Publishing Group}, title = {{A functional and perceptual signature of the second visual area in primates}}, url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3402}, volume = {16}, year = {2013} } @Article{Freeman2013, author={Freeman, Jeremy and Ziemba, Corey M. and Heeger, David J. and Simoncelli, Eero P. and Movshon, J. Anthony}, title={A functional and perceptual signature of the second visual area in primates}, journal={Nature Neuroscience}, year={2013}, month={Jul}, day={01}, volume={16}, number={7}, pages={974-981}, abstract={The authors examined neuronal responses in V1 and V2 to synthetic texture stimuli that replicate higher-order statistical dependencies found in natural images. V2, but not V1, responded differentially to these textures, in both macaque (single neurons) and human (fMRI). Human detection of naturalistic structure in the same images was predicted by V2 responses, suggesting a role for V2 in representing natural image structure.}, issn={1546-1726}, doi={10.1038/nn.3402}, url={https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3402} } @misc{Sanghavi_Jozwik_DiCarlo_2021, title={SanghaviJozwik2020}, url={osf.io/fhy36}, DOI={10.17605/OSF.IO/FHY36}, publisher={OSF}, author={Sanghavi, Sachi and Jozwik, Kamila M and DiCarlo, James J}, year={2021}, month={Nov} } @misc{Sanghavi_DiCarlo_2021, title={Sanghavi2020}, url={osf.io/chwdk}, DOI={10.17605/OSF.IO/CHWDK}, publisher={OSF}, author={Sanghavi, Sachi and DiCarlo, James J}, year={2021}, month={Nov} } @misc{Sanghavi_Murty_DiCarlo_2021, title={SanghaviMurty2020}, url={osf.io/fchme}, DOI={10.17605/OSF.IO/FCHME}, publisher={OSF}, author={Sanghavi, Sachi and Murty, N A R and DiCarlo, James J}, year={2021}, month={Nov} } @article {Majaj13402, author = {Majaj, Najib J. and Hong, Ha and Solomon, Ethan A. and DiCarlo, James J.}, title = {Simple Learned Weighted Sums of Inferior Temporal Neuronal Firing Rates Accurately Predict Human Core Object Recognition Performance}, volume = {35}, number = {39}, pages = {13402--13418}, year = {2015}, doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5181-14.2015}, publisher = {Society for Neuroscience}, abstract = {To go beyond qualitative models of the biological substrate of object recognition, we ask: can a single ventral stream neuronal linking hypothesis quantitatively account for core object recognition performance over a broad range of tasks? We measured human performance in 64 object recognition tests using thousands of challenging images that explore shape similarity and identity preserving object variation. We then used multielectrode arrays to measure neuronal population responses to those same images in visual areas V4 and inferior temporal (IT) cortex of monkeys and simulated V1 population responses. We tested leading candidate linking hypotheses and control hypotheses, each postulating how ventral stream neuronal responses underlie object recognition behavior. Specifically, for each hypothesis, we computed the predicted performance on the 64 tests and compared it with the measured pattern of human performance. All tested hypotheses based on low- and mid-level visually evoked activity (pixels, V1, and V4) were very poor predictors of the human behavioral pattern. However, simple learned weighted sums of distributed average IT firing rates exactly predicted the behavioral pattern. More elaborate linking hypotheses relying on IT trial-by-trial correlational structure, finer IT temporal codes, or ones that strictly respect the known spatial substructures of IT ({ extquotedblleft}face patches{ extquotedblright}) did not improve predictive power. Although these results do not reject those more elaborate hypotheses, they suggest a simple, sufficient quantitative model: each object recognition task is learned from the spatially distributed mean firing rates (100 ms) of \~{}60,000 IT neurons and is executed as a simple weighted sum of those firing rates.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We sought to go beyond qualitative models of visual object recognition and determine whether a single neuronal linking hypothesis can quantitatively account for core object recognition behavior. To achieve this, we designed a database of images for evaluating object recognition performance. We used multielectrode arrays to characterize hundreds of neurons in the visual ventral stream of nonhuman primates and measured the object recognition performance of \>100 human observers. Remarkably, we found that simple learned weighted sums of firing rates of neurons in monkey inferior temporal (IT) cortex accurately predicted human performance. Although previous work led us to expect that IT would outperform V4, we were surprised by the quantitative precision with which simple IT-based linking hypotheses accounted for human behavior.}, issn = {0270-6474}, URL = {https://www.jneurosci.org/content/35/39/13402}, eprint = {https://www.jneurosci.org/content/35/39/13402.full.pdf}, journal = {Journal of Neuroscience}} @Article{Kar2019, author={Kar, Kohitij and Kubilius, Jonas and Schmidt, Kailyn and Issa, Elias B. and DiCarlo, James J.}, title={Evidence that recurrent circuits are critical to the ventral stream's execution of core object recognition behavior}, journal={Nature Neuroscience}, year={2019}, month={Jun}, day={01}, volume={22}, number={6}, pages={974-983}, abstract={Non-recurrent deep convolutional neural networks (CNNs) are currently the best at modeling core object recognition, a behavior that is supported by the densely recurrent primate ventral stream, culminating in the inferior temporal (IT) cortex. If recurrence is critical to this behavior, then primates should outperform feedforward-only deep CNNs for images that require additional recurrent processing beyond the feedforward IT response. Here we first used behavioral methods to discover hundreds of these `challenge' images. Second, using large-scale electrophysiology, we observed that behaviorally sufficient object identity solutions emerged { extasciitilde}30{ hinspace}ms later in the IT cortex for challenge images compared with primate performance-matched `control' images. Third, these behaviorally critical late-phase IT response patterns were poorly predicted by feedforward deep CNN activations. Notably, very-deep CNNs and shallower recurrent CNNs better predicted these late IT responses, suggesting that there is a functional equivalence between additional nonlinear transformations and recurrence. Beyond arguing that recurrent circuits are critical for rapid object identification, our results provide strong constraints for future recurrent model development.}, issn={1546-1726}, doi={10.1038/s41593-019-0392-5}, url={https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0392-5} } @article {Rajalingham240614, author = {Rajalingham, Rishi and Issa, Elias B. and Bashivan, Pouya and Kar, Kohitij and Schmidt, Kailyn and DiCarlo, James J.}, title = {Large-scale, high-resolution comparison of the core visual object recognition behavior of humans, monkeys, and state-of-the-art deep artificial neural networks}, elocation-id = {240614}, year = {2018}, doi = {10.1101/240614}, publisher = {Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory}, abstract = {Primates{ extemdash}including humans{ extemdash}can typically recognize objects in visual images at a glance even in the face of naturally occurring identity-preserving image transformations (e.g. changes in viewpoint). A primary neuroscience goal is to uncover neuron-level mechanistic models that quantitatively explain this behavior by predicting primate performance for each and every image. Here, we applied this stringent behavioral prediction test to the leading mechanistic models of primate vision (specifically, deep, convolutional, artificial neural networks; ANNs) by directly comparing their behavioral signatures against those of humans and rhesus macaque monkeys. Using high-throughput data collection systems for human and monkey psychophysics, we collected over one million behavioral trials for 2400 images over 276 binary object discrimination tasks. Consistent with previous work, we observed that state-of-the-art deep, feed-forward convolutional ANNs trained for visual categorization (termed DCNNIC models) accurately predicted primate patterns of object-level confusion. However, when we examined behavioral performance for individual images within each object discrimination task, we found that all tested DCNNIC models were significantly non-predictive of primate performance, and that this prediction failure was not accounted for by simple image attributes, nor rescued by simple model modifications. These results show that current DCNNIC models cannot account for the image-level behavioral patterns of primates, and that new ANN models are needed to more precisely capture the neural mechanisms underlying primate object vision. To this end, large-scale, high-resolution primate behavioral benchmarks{ extemdash}such as those obtained here{ extemdash}could serve as direct guides for discovering such models.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Recently, specific feed-forward deep convolutional artificial neural networks (ANNs) models have dramatically advanced our quantitative understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying primate core object recognition. In this work, we tested the limits of those ANNs by systematically comparing the behavioral responses of these models with the behavioral responses of humans and monkeys, at the resolution of individual images. Using these high-resolution metrics, we found that all tested ANN models significantly diverged from primate behavior. Going forward, these high-resolution, large-scale primate behavioral benchmarks could serve as direct guides for discovering better ANN models of the primate visual system.}, URL = {https://www.biorxiv.org/content/early/2018/02/12/240614}, eprint = {https://www.biorxiv.org/content/early/2018/02/12/240614.full.pdf}, journal = {bioRxiv} } @article{geirhos2021partial, title={Partial success in closing the gap between human and machine vision}, author={Geirhos, Robert and Narayanappa, Kantharaju and Mitzkus, Benjamin and Thieringer, Tizian and Bethge, Matthias and Wichmann, Felix A and Brendel, Wieland}, journal={Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems}, volume={34}, year={2021}, url={https://openreview.net/forum?id=QkljT4mrfs} } @INPROCEEDINGS{5206848, author={J. {Deng} and W. {Dong} and R. {Socher} and L. {Li} and {Kai Li} and {Li Fei-Fei}}, booktitle={2009 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition}, title={ImageNet: A large-scale hierarchical image database}, year={2009}, volume={}, number={}, pages={248-255}, } @ARTICLE{Hendrycks2019-di, title = "Benchmarking Neural Network Robustness to Common Corruptions and Perturbations", author = "Hendrycks, Dan and Dietterich, Thomas", abstract = "In this paper we establish rigorous benchmarks for image classifier robustness. Our first benchmark, ImageNet-C, standardizes and expands the corruption robustness topic, while showing which classifiers are preferable in safety-critical applications. Then we propose a new dataset called ImageNet-P which enables researchers to benchmark a classifier's robustness to common perturbations. Unlike recent robustness research, this benchmark evaluates performance on common corruptions and perturbations not worst-case adversarial perturbations. We find that there are negligible changes in relative corruption robustness from AlexNet classifiers to ResNet classifiers. Afterward we discover ways to enhance corruption and perturbation robustness. We even find that a bypassed adversarial defense provides substantial common perturbation robustness. Together our benchmarks may aid future work toward networks that robustly generalize.", month = mar, year = 2019, archivePrefix = "arXiv", primaryClass = "cs.LG", eprint = "1903.12261", url = "https://arxiv.org/abs/1903.12261" } @inproceedings{DBLP:conf/nips/BarbuMALWGTK19, author = {Andrei Barbu and David Mayo and Julian Alverio and William Luo and Christopher Wang and Dan Gutfreund and Josh Tenenbaum and Boris Katz}, title = {ObjectNet: {A} large-scale bias-controlled dataset for pushing the limits of object recognition models}, booktitle = {NeurIPS 2019}, pages = {9448--9458}, year = {2019}, url = {https://proceedings.neurips.cc/paper/2019/hash/97af07a14cacba681feacf3012730892-Abstract.html}, }